The Competitiveness of Nations in a Global Knowledge-Based Economy
Robin Cowan (a), Paul A.
David (b) & Dominique Foray (c)
The Explicit Economics of Knowledge: Codifcation and Tacitness
Content |
|
Abstract
1. Introduction: What’s All this Fuss over Tacit
Knowledge About?
2. How the Tacit Dimension Found a Wonderful New
Career in Economics
2.1 The Roots in the Sociology of Scientific
Knowledge, and Cognitive Science
2.2 From Evolutionary Economics to Management Strategy
and Technology Policy 3. Codification and Tacitness Reconsidered 4. A Proposed Topography for Knowledge Activities 5. Boundaries in the Re-mapped Knowledge Space and
Their Significance 6. On the Value of This Re-mapping 6.1 On the Topography Itself 6.2 On Interactions with External Phenomena |
7. The Economic Determinants of Codification 7.1 The Endogeneity of the Tacitness - Codification
Boundary 7.2 Costs, Benefits and the Knowledge Environment 7.3 Costs and Benefits in a Stable Context 7.4 Costs and Benefits in the Context of Change 8. Conclusions and the Direction of Further Work Acknowledgements References HHC: Index added
Industrial and Corporate Change, 9 (2), 2000 , 211-253 |
page 3
5.
Boundaries in the Re-mapped Knowledge Space and Their Significance
Across the space described by the foregoing taxonomic
structure it is possible to define (at least) three interesting boundaries. The ‘Collins-Latour-Callon’ boundary would
separate articulated codified knowledge from all the rest - assigning
observational situations in which there was a displaced codebook to the same
realm as that in which learning and transmission of scientific knowledge, and praxis,
were proceeding in the absence of codification. The line labeled ‘the Merton-Kuhn boundary’
puts codified and codebook-displaced situations together on its left side, and
would focus primary attention there - as it constituted the distinctive regions
occupied by modern science. That would
leave all the rest to general psychological and sociological inquiries about
‘enculturation processes’ involved in human knowledge acquisition.
Our branching structure recognizes that it is possible,
nonetheless, for epistemic communities to exist and function in the zone
to the right of the Merton-Kuhn boundary. Such communities, which may be small working
groups, comprise knowledge-creating agents who are engaged on a mutually
recognized subset of questions, and who (at the very least) accept some
commonly understood procedural authority as essential to the success of their
collective activities. The line labeled
the ‘functional epistemic community boundary’ separates them from the region
immediately to their right in the topography. Beyond that border lies the zone populated by
personal (and organizational) gurus of one shape or another, including the ‘new
age’ cult
234
leaders in whom
procedural and personal authority over the conduct of group affairs are fused.
As
is clear from the foregoing discussion, there are two quite distinct aspects of
knowledge that are pertinent in the codified-tacit discussions, although they
are often left unidentified. On the one
hand, knowledge might or might not be presented or stored in a text. This is the notion associated with
codification. On the other hand, there
is the degree to which knowledge appears explicitly in standard activities. Here, we can think of knowledge as being manifest
or not. Figure 2 elaborates these
two properties in a tableau. For this
purpose we have used a 3 X 3 matrix, in which one axis represents the extent of
codification: codified, partially codified, and uncodified; the other axis
represents the extent to which the knowledge is manifest, or commonly referred
to in knowledge endeavors: manifest, alluded to, and latent. These divisions that are indicated along the
vertical and horizontal axes are patently arbitrary, for mixtures in the
ordinary human knowledge activities form a continuum, rather than a set of
discrete boxes.
To
make clearer the meaning of Figure 2, it may be useful to look specifically at
the four extreme cases: the corners north-west, south-west, north-east and
south-east. Both the codified-manifest
case (the north-west corner) and the uncodified-latent case (the south-east
corner) describe situations which are easily comprehensible because the
criteria fit together
235
naturally.
The codified-latent case (the north-east)
was described as a situation in which the codebook is displaced while knowledge
is not tacit. Finally the uncodified-manifest
case (south-west) describes situations in which agents start to make their
discoveries, inventions and new ideas manifest (in order to diffuse them) but
still cannot use a full and stabilized codebook to do so; and even ‘writing’ a
book with which to make this new knowledge manifest does not necessarily imply
codification. It is possible that the
vocabulary or symbolic representation employed is highly idiosyncratic, that
there are many ambiguities, and so on. This
implies that while certain aspects of codification may be present (in knowledge
storage and recall, for example), other important aspects (such as an agreed
vocabulary of expression) can be missing.
The overly sharp coordinates nevertheless give us a tableau
which can be used heuristically in distinguishing major regions of the
states-space within which knowledge-groups can be working at a given moments in
their history. Instruction or deliberate
knowledge transfer is thus roughly situated in the tableau’s ‘manifest’ column,
spilling over somewhat into the ‘alluded to’ column. Formal instruction comes near the top
(‘codified’), whereas apprenticeship lies near the bottom (‘uncodified’) of the
array. The world of normal science
inquiries extends across the ellipse-shaped region that is oriented along the
minor diagonal (southwest-northeast axis) of the array, leaving out the
north-west and south-west corners. In
the former of the two excluded regions codified knowledge is most plainly
manifested for purposes of didactic instruction, in which reference is made to
textbooks, grammars and dictionaries, manuals, reference standards, and the
like. In contrast, most craft
apprenticeship training, and even some computer-based tutorial programs, occupy
the lower portion of the ‘soft-trapezoidal’ region covering the left side of
the array in Figure 2: a mixture of both codified and uncodified procedural
routines are made manifest, through the use of manuals as well as practical
demonstrations, whereas other bodies of codified knowledge may simply be
alluded to in the course of instruction. [20]
The boundaries of the world of engineering and applied
R&D also extend upwards from the south-west corner in which closed,
proprietary research
20. A number of
interesting examples are presented in Balconi’s (1998) study of worker training
programs in the modern steel-making industry, showing that what formerly could
justifiably be described as ‘rules of the art’ have been transformed into
codified knowledge of a generic sort, as well as explicit operating procedures
for the plant in question. According to
Balconi (pp. 73-74), an overly sharp distinction has been drawn by Bell and
Pavitt (1993) when they contrast the nature of the ‘learning within firms’ that
is necessary to augment the content of the formal education and training
conducted by institutions outside industry. In the cases she discusses, ‘the aim of
training [provided within the industry] is to transmit know-how by teaching
know-why (the explanations of the causes of the physical transformations
carried out [in the plant]), and know-what (codified operation practices)’.
236
groups
function on the basis of the uncodified skills (experience-based expertise) of
the team members, and their shared and manifest references to procedures that
previously were found to be successful. But,
if we are to accept the descriptions provided for us by articulate academic
engineers, those boundaries are more tightly drawn than the ones within which
science-groups operate; in particular they do not reach as far upwards into the
area where there is a large body of latent but nevertheless highly codified
knowledge under-girding research and discovery (see, e.g., Vincenti, 1990;
Ferguson, 1992).
6.
On the Value of This Re-mapping
What value can be claimed for the topography of knowledge
activities that we have just presented? Evidently
it serves to increase the precision and to allow greater nuance in the
distinctions made among types of knowledge-getting and transferring pursuits. But, in addition, and of greater usefulness
for purposes of economic analysis, it will be seen to permit a more fruitful
examination of the influence of external, economic conditions upon the codification
and manifestation of knowledge as information. A number of the specific benefits derived from
looking at the world in this way warrant closer examination, and to these we
now turn.
Figures 1 and 2 clean up a confusion concerning the putative
tacitness of the working knowledge of scientists in situations that we have
here been able to characterize by applying the ‘displaced codebook’ rubric. A number of studies, including some widely cited
in the literature, seem to have placed undue reliance upon an overly casual
observational test, identifying situations where no codebook was manifestly
present as instances showing the crucial role of ‘tacit knowledge’, pure and
simple (see e.g. Collins, 1974; Latour and Woolgar, 1979; Latour, 1987;
Traweek, 1988). It now should be seen
that this fails to allow for the possibility that explicit references to
codified sources of ‘authority’ may be supplanted by the formation of ‘common
knowledge’ regarding the subscription of the epistemic community to that
displaced but nonetheless ‘authoritative’ body of information.
The location of the Collins-Latour-Callon boundary in Figure
1, and its relationship to the regeneration of knowledge in tacit form,
signifies that this latter process - involving the mental ‘repackaging’ of
formal algorithms and other codified materials for more efficient retrieval and
frequent applications,
237
including
those involved in the recombinant creation of new knowledge - rests upon the
pre-existing establishment of a well-articulated body of codified, disciplinary
tools. [21]
Economists’ recent concerns with the economics of knowledge
tend to lie in the ‘no disagreements’ (uncodified, manifest) box in Figure 2. We are talking here about the literature that
views reliance upon ‘sticky data’ or ‘local jargons’ as methods of
appropriating returns from knowledge. This
is the region of the map in which knowledge-building gives rise to the ‘quasi’
aspect of the description of knowledge assets as quasi-public goods. The immediate implication is that to determine
the degree to which some particular pieces of knowledge are indeed only
quasi-public goods calls for a contextual examination of both the completeness
of the codification and the extent of manifestation.
The argument has recently been advanced, by Gibbons et
al. (1996), that an emergent late twentieth century trend was the rise of a
new regime of knowledge production, so-called Mode 2. This has been contrasted with the antecedent
dominant organizational features of scientific inquiry, associated with Mode 1:
in particular, the new regime is described as being more reliant upon tacit
knowledge, and transdisciplinary - as opposed to the importance accorded by
Mode 1 to the publication of codified material in areas of disciplinary
specialization as the legitimate basis of collegiate reputational status,
selection decisions in personnel recruitment, and the structuring of criteria
for evaluating and rewarding professional performance. Doubts have been raised about the alleged
novelty and self-sufficiency of Mode 2 as a successor that will displace the
antecedent, highly institutionalized system of research and innovation (see
e.g. David et al, 1999, and references therein). But the main point to be noted in the present
context is that such coherence and functionality as groups working in Mode 2
have been able to obtain would appear to rest upon their development of
procedural authority to which the fluid membership subscribes.
6.2 On
Interactions with External Phenomena
How do changes in information and communications technologies
impinge
21. Further, in much the
same vein, it is quite possible that practiced experimental researchers, having
developed and codified procedures involving a sequence of discrete steps, may
be observed discussing and executing the routine in a holistic manner - even to
the point of finding it difficult to immediately articulate every discrete
constituent step of the process. The
latter is a situation found quite commonly when experienced computer
programmers are asked to explain and document the strings of code that they
have just typed. The phenomenon would
seem to have more to do with the efficient ‘granularity’ for mental storage
and recall of knowledge, than with the nature of the knowledge itself, or
the manner in which it was initially acquired.
238
upon
the distribution of knowledge production and distribution activities within the
re-mapped space described by our topography? The first and most obvious thing to notice is
the endogeneity of the boundaries (which we discuss more fully in the next
section). In the new taxonomy there are
two interesting distinctions: knowledge activities may use and produce codified
or uncodified knowledge, or they may use and produce knowledge that is either
manifest or latent. We should re-state
here that ‘boundary’ is not being used in reference to the distribution of the
world knowledge stock, but instead to the prevailing locus of the activities of
knowledge agents in a specific cognitive, temporal and social milieu. Nevertheless, it is most likely to be true
that the situation of a group’s knowledge stock will be intimately related to,
and possibly even coterminous with, the location of its knowledge production
activities.
Organizational goals affect the manifest-latent boundary. Activities that couple teaching with research,
for example, will be pushed towards the more fully ‘manifest’ region of the
state space. This consideration will be important
in studies of the economics of science, and of the institutional organization
of academic research activities more generally. [22]
The positioning of the endogenously determined boundary
separating the codified from the uncodified states of knowledge activities will
be governed by the following three sets of forces, which we examine at greater
length below. For the present it is
sufficient simply to note that these include: (i) costs and benefits of the
activity of codification; (ii) the costs and benefits of the use of the
codified knowledge (data compression, transmission, storage, retrieval,
management, etc.); and (iii) feedbacks that arise because of the way codified
knowledge is used to generate further codified knowledge.
A given discipline’s age (or the stage of development
reached in the life cycle of a particular area of specialization) affects both
of the boundaries. The evolution of a
discipline, a technological domain (or of a research group or a community of
practitioners) may now be described as a movement in the two-dimensional plane
of the tableau in Figure 2. Let us
illustrate this by taking a lead from Kuhn (1962) and begin, early in the life
cycle of a research program, with activities centered in the south-east corner
of the array: a disparate collection of individual researchers and small teams,
working without any commonly accepted procedural authority, generating
knowledge that remains highly idiosyncratic and uncodified, and having only a
very restricted scope for transmission of its findings beyond the confines of
the immediate work-group(s). Subsequently,
if and when these investigations
22. A recent
exemplification of the application of the approach formalized here is available
in Geuna’s (1999) studies of the economics of university funding for research
in science and engineering, and how different funding structures affect the
types of activity within the modern European university system.
239
have
begun bearing fruit, work in the field coalesces around a more compact set of
questions and the locus of its knowledge activities shifts westward, as agents
make their discoveries and inventions manifest either in physical artifacts,
conference presentations and published papers. This is where the codification process begins.
But even though scholarly texts may be
produced, because the concepts involved and language in which these reports are
couched have not yet been thoroughly standardized, codification must still be
considered very incomplete. Thenceforward,
the emerging discipline’s course follows a northerly track, spreading towards
the north-east as disputes arise from inconsistencies in description and
interpretation, and conflicts emerge over the specifics of the way the language
is to be standardized. As these debates
are resolved and closure on a widening range of issues is achieved, the
characteristic activities migrate eastward in the space of Figure 2, landing up
in the region where most of the actual research activity is carried on within
the ‘latent-codified’ and ‘manifest-partially codified’ domains that typify
normal science.